Volume 12, No. 3 
July 2008

 
 

Shih Chung-ling


 
 

Front Page

 
 
 
Select one of the previous 44 issues.

 

Index 1997-2008

 
TJ Interactive: Translation Journal Blog

 
  Translator Profiles
On Becoming a Translator
by Salvador Virgen

 
  The Profession
The Bottom Line
by Fire Ant & Worker Bee
 
Everything’s Comin’ up Roses (with apologies to Stephen Sondheim)
by Bernie Bierman
 
Navigating in a New Era: Translators in the Age of Image and Speech
by Eileen B. Hennessy
 
Supply and Demand Analysis of Patent Translation
by Yvonne Tsai

 
  In Memoriam
A Farewell to Vera—In Memoriam Vera Maria Conti Nogueira: 1944 - 2008
by Danilo Nogueira

 
  Nuts and Bolts of Translation
Übersetzung deutscher Nominalkomposita aus der Fachsprache der Technik und Analyse typischer portugiesischer Entsprechungen
Katrin Herget, Holger Proschwitz
 
Proper Names and Translation
by Samira Mizani

 
  Translators Around the World
The Influence of the Market on Translating—A Tentative Study of the Market-oriented Translation in China
by Tian Chuanmao

 
  Scientific and Technical Translation
Mini-Guide to Translating French Documents
for English-Speaking Markets (with general tips for other language pairs and writers of EFL)

by M.L. Seren-Rosso

 
  Cultural Aspects of Translation
Translating Sexuality: The Translation Industry and Adult Websites
by Sathya Rao

 
  Literary Translation
Corpus-based Study of Differences in Explicitation Between Literature Translations for Children and for Adults
by Shih Chung-ling

 
  Translator Education
Bibliografía comentada sobre Traducción e Interpretación para estudiantes
Pablo Muñoz Sánchez
 
Individual Differences in the Translation Process: Differences in the act of translation between two groups of ESL Japanese students
by Atsushi Iida
 
El análisis crítico de traducciones literarias en la formación de traductores
Dra. Beatriz MĒ Rodríguez Rodríguez

 
  Book Reviews
Book Review: A Companion to Translation Studies
by Esmaeil Haddadian Moghaddam
 
Book Review: The Locas mujeres poems of Gabriela Mistral
reviewed by Liliana Valenzuela

 
  Translation Theory
Meaning: The Philosopher's Stone of the Alchemist Translator?
by Maite Aragonés Lumeras, Ph.D.

 
  Translators' Tools
Translators’ Emporium

 
  Caught in the Web
Web Surfing for Fun and Profit
by Cathy Flick, Ph.D.
 
Translation and Participatory Media: Experiences from Global Voices
by Chris Salzberg
 
Translators’ On-Line Resources
by Gabe Bokor
 
Translators’ Best Websites
by Gabe Bokor

 
Call for Papers and Editorial Policies
  Translation Journal


Literary Translation

Corpus-based Study of Differences in Explicitation

Between Literature Translations for Children and for Adults

by Shih Chung-ling
English Dept. of National Kaohsiung First University of Science and Technology


Abstract

This article investigates the differences in explicitation between English-to-Chinese literature translations for children and for adults using the corpus-based analysis method. Explicitation is the overall tendency to explicate implicit messages in translation. The assumption is that literature translation for children shows a higher percentage of explicitation than for adults because children readers require the explicitly-presented logical context and repetitive lexical items for easy comprehension and easy memorization. My inference is that the differences in explicitation result from the pursuit of literary function in the translation for adults and educational function in the translation for children. Reduced noun variation, clear inter/intra-sentential relationship and more notes help children to learn words easily, catch the message and acquire cultural and specialized knowledge. In contrast, the higher variation in lexicon, fewer added connectives, and fewer added notes create literary beauty and boost the adult reader's aesthetic appreciation. The research finding concurs with Hans J. Vermeer's skopos theory and Zohar Shavit's poetics of children's literature that explicitaiton degree varies when target language audience and translation purposes change. In sum, this research serves as a model of purpose-oriented explicitation investigation and can be replicated for future researches on other literary genres, e.g. science writing, in other language pairs.

Keywords: explicitation, literature translation, noun density, added connectives, added notes, translation purposes

 

I. Introduction

n this study, I will examine the explication variation between translated novels for children and for adults using the corpus-based analysis method. Explicitation refers to an overall tendency to "spell things out rather than leave them implicit in translation" and has been identified as one of the universal translational features through corpus-based translation studies (CTS) (Baker 1996, p.180). CTS was initiated by Mona Baker in 1990s and has been undertaken to investigate the recurrent features of "explicitation, simplification, normalization/conventionality and leveling out" (Baker 1996, p. 84). This study simply focuses on the feature of explicitation by analyzing two English-to-Chinese parallel literary corpora1. The purpose is to observe how literature translations for children and for adults show different degrees of noun density2 added connectives and added notes. Since children and adults read for different purposes, translators will use different lexicons and different sentence structures, either simple or complicated, to produce varied degrees of explicitation and fulfill different translation purposes.

The degree of explicitation varies between translations of children and adults' literature.
A literature review of current CTSs leads us to conclude that an investigation into explicitation tends to be text-genre and language-pairs-oriented, and overlooks the relevance of explicitaiton variation to extra-textual factors of different target readers and different translation purposes. Many CTS scholars investigated either the translations of different text genres (e.g. Séquinot, 1988; Baker, 1992) or those in various language pairs (e.g. Shlesinger, 1995; Kulka, 1986) to justify the feature of explicitation, but rarely delved into differences in explicitation resulting from the translations of the same genre for different purposes. In actuality, audiences and purposes are crucial factors in determining different translation strategies and different degrees of explicitation in the translations of the same genre. For example, literature translation for children is intended to teach children new words or to strengthen their reading ability in the target language (TL), while literature translation for adults is intended to provide aesthetic appreciation and spiritual amusement. These different purposes will result in different levels of explicitation in the translated novels for children and for adults.

Rarely do we avoid the issue of readability when discussing literature translation for children. To enhance the level of readability for children, translated novels for children must have an overall higher level of explicitation than translated novels for adults because children require translations to be more easily comprehended than adults do. Many people will agree with this assumption, but assumptions still require an empirical test for genuine assurance. At the onset of this test, I raise several questions for an investigation.

  1. Does literature translation for children show a lower percentage of noun density than that for adults and therefore have a higher degree of explicitation?
  2. Does literature translation for children show a higher frequency of added connectives than that for adults and therefore have a higher degree of explicitation?
  3. Does literature translation for children show a higher frequency of added notes than that for adults and therefore have a higher degree of explicitation?
  4. How is the explicitiaton variation relevant to different translation purposes for different types of target readers?

Answers to these questions are expected to verify the purpose-oriented assumption of the explicitation differences between literature translations for children and for adults. Since this study uses Hans J. Vermeer's skopos theory and Zohar Shavit's poetics of children's literature as the theoretical framework for argument, I have to introduce their main concepts at some length in the following section. After that, I will discuss the corpus-based analysis method, including corpus design and search criteria. In addition, I will report some findings and discuss the implications of these findings to elicit significant insights. Finally, I will summarize this study, describe its limitations and suggest some possibilities for future research.


II. Theoretical Review

This research mainly explores the relevance of differences in explicitation to different translation purposes for children and for adults, so the theories of explicitation, translation purpose and the translation poetics of children's literature will be discussed as follows.

2.1. Explicitation Research

The term "explicitation" first appeared in Vinay and Darbelnet's article ([1958]1995; qtd. in Klaudy 1998, p. 80). Vinay and Darbelnet (1958) define explicitation as "the process of introducing information into the target language which is present only implicit in the source language, but which can be derived from the context or the situation" (qtd. in Klaudy 1998, p. 80). In other words, explicitation is "the process of introducing information into the target language which is present only implicitly in the source language, but which [could] be derived from the context or situation" (ibid.). E. A. Nida (1964) looks at the explicitation phenomenon in the translated text as a shift from the implicit to the explicit status that occurs when "important semantic elements carried implicitly in the source language may require explicit identification in the receptor language" (p. 228; qtd. in Klaudy 1998, p. 81).

There are abundant corpus-based studies of explicitation. S. Blum-Kulka conducted a systematic discourse analysis of English-French translations and English-Hebrew translations and found a noticeable rise in the level of cohesive explicitness from source texts to translated texts (cited here in a 2000 reprint, p. 300). To test the explicitation hypothesis, L. Øverås (1998) investigated the bi-directional English-Norwegian Parallel Corpus and discovered that the level of explicitness in both translated English and translated Norwegian was greater than their originals. M. Olohan and M. Baker (2000) conducted a comparable study on the addition of the word that after the verbs say and tell, with the aid of two corpora, Translational English Corpus (TEC) and British National Corpus (BNC). The analytical result showed that the use of that with say and tell in translational English corpus was more frequent than in non-translational English corpus. Moreover, M. Mutesayire (2005) used a comparable corpus to investigate reformulation markers such as in other words, namely, that is to say, and to be more precise/specific. These lexical items were found to occur more frequently in translated English than non-translated English (Olahan 2003).

All these studies confirmed the universality of explicitation that was produced independently of the impact of language pairs and text types in the translation process. These studies have found the global pattern that explicitation was produced not only without interference from language pairs but also without much relevance to text types. However, we cannot overlook potential local differences existing behind the generalization of explicitation when we consider the factors of different translation purposes. Thus, this study will examine how the explicitation feature shows different degrees between literature translations for children and for adults because the novels translated for these two types of audiences are expected to meet different reading purposes.


2.2. The Skopos Theory

Vermeer's skopos theory argues that "every translation can and must be assigned a skopos" (1989, p. 230). The Greek word, "skopos" refers to the purpose of any piece of translation (Baker 2001; Munday 2001). In Vermeer's view, translating is not only a process of linguistic transfer from one language to another, but the cross-cultural transfer for the purpose of effective communication. The translation performance cannot be appropriate and satisfying if the translator is unaware of the purpose and the prospective readers of the translation.

Vermeer regards the addressee of the target language (TL) text as the main factor in determining the skopos (purpose) of translation (ibid.). "The skopos can be said to vary according to the recipient" (Reiss & Vermeer, 1984, p. 101). It follows the principle that "the translator should use the translation strategies which are most appropriate for achieving the purpose for which TT is intended" (Shuttleworth & Cowie, 1997, p.156). For example, to increase the child reader's knowledge in certain domains, the translator supplies background information of certain proper nouns with added notes for educational purpose. In addition, added cultural notes can be effective communication because cultural references in the source language (SL) text are situated in the source culture and henceforth require additional explanations to ensure the TL reader's comprehension. This example suggests that the intended purpose of translation affects the translator's decision-making and determines his/her translation strategies.


2.3. Poetics of Children's Literature

Due to different TL audiences and different translation purposes, a SL text can be translated in several ways. In her poetics of children's literature, Shavit (1986) proposes a set of rules for children's literature translation as opposed to those for adult literature translation. Shavit (1986) maintains that literature translation for children can follow two principles for two purposes. One is "an adjustment of the text to make it appropriate and useful to the child in accordance with what society regards (at a certain point in time) as educationally 'good for the child'" (Shavit 1986, p. 113). The other is "an adjustment of plot, characterization, and language to society's perceptions of the child's ability to read and comprehend" (ibid.). These two principles denote the importance of high readability, easy comprehensibility and didactic usefulness for child readers.

In contrast, to meet the need of adult readers, Shavit proposes the high literary style for the effect of "literariness" per se after comparing Hebrew literature written for children and for adults (1968, p. 128). She declares that the translator can use the special technique of linguistic presentation to create literary beauty and meet adult readers' need for aesthetic appreciation. Shavit's theory resembles Vermeer's skopos theory because both of them agree that the translator should use different methods and techniques to achieve different purposes.


III. Methodology

This explicitation investigation expects to gain a more objective and more reliable conclusion through the corpus-based analysis of a huge volume of authentic data retrieved from the real market. Besides, this study includes a statistical survey with the aid of corpus-processing tools.3 However, it is crucial to have appropriate corpus design and set up search criteria prior to the investigation if it aims at obtaining complete and relevant results. These two components will be introduced as follows.


3.1. Corpus Design

In this study, two parallel corpora of literature translations are used as analytical data. One is composed of the first chapters of five English novels with their Chinese translations for children and contains a total of 24,175 words (hereinafter Literature Parallel Corpus for Children/LPCC). The other is composed of the first chapters of the same five English novels with their Chinese translations for adults and contains a total of 108,386 words (hereinafter Literature Parallel Corpus for Adults/LPCA). A combination of these two corpora has a total of 132,561 words. The translated Chinese novels for children contain much fewer words than the translated novels for adults because literature translation for children, motivated by the need to make the text more communicative and more understandable, involves much adaptation but transmits similar messages. Tables 1 and 2 show the internal structures of LPCC and LPCA.


Table 1

The Internal Structure of LPCC

English Novels

Authors

Translation Titles

Translators

Words

Gulliver's Travels

J. Swift

小人國和大人國

[Dwarf & Giant Kingdoms]

盧潔峰

SL: 2355

TL: 2477

Robinson Crusoe

D. Defoe

魯賓遜漂流記

[Robinson's Adventures]

周樂

SL: 2628

TL: 1968

A Tale of Two Cities

C. Dickens

雙城記

[A Tale of Two Cities]

趙小敏

SL: 1700

TL: 2145

Treasure Island

R. L. Stevenson

金銀島

[Golden-Silver Island]

盧潔峰

SL: 2312

TL: 2494

The Adventures of Huckleberry Finn

M. Twain

小哈克奇遇記

[Little Huck's Adventures]

周樂

SL: 2648

TL: 3448

 

 

 

 

Total: 24,175


Table 2

The Internal Structure of LPCA

English Novels

Authors

Translation Titles

Translators

Words

Gulliver's Travels

J. Swift

格列佛遊記

[Gulliver's Travels]

史曉麗,王林

SL: 4104

TL: 5402

Robinson Crusoe

D. Defoe

魯賓遜漂流記

[Robinson's Adventures]

鹿金

SL: 14878

TL: 24610

A Tale of Two Cities

C. Dickens

雙城記

[A Tale of Two Cities]

文怡虹

SL: 9486

TL: 17281

Treasure Island

R. L. Stevenson

金銀島

[Golden-Silver Island]

黃建

SL: 6814

TL: 11224

The Adventures of Huckleberry Finn

M. Twain

頑童流浪記

[A Naughty Boy's Adventures]

賈文浩,

賈文淵

SL: 5752

TL: 8835

 

 

 

 

Total: 108,386


A comparison of Table 1 and Table 2 shows that the five translated novels for children were published in the late 1990s4 while the same novels translated for adults were published between 2001 and 2006.5 The reason for this gap is that Taiwan's translated novels for children with added notes were published in the late 1990s while Taiwan's translated novels for adults with added notes were not published until the first decade of the 2000s. Noticeably, the five novels are selected mainly because their translations for children and for adults are available on Taiwan's market.


3.2. Search Criteria

Following a study of the noun-density-resulted explicitation, I select certain types of nouns as key words for search. In addition, the explicitation phenomena generated from added connectives at the structural level and added notes at the contextual level are probed, so that certain connectives and culture-specific notes are selected for search.

3.2.1. Noun Types

The three types of nouns to be selected are general nouns, proper nouns and pronouns. Some of them are listed in Table 3.


Table 3

Three Noun Types

Noun Types

Lexical Items

General Nouns

父親 [father]、醫生[doctor]、親戚[relatives]、費用 [expense]、恩師 [good master]、船長[captain]、朋友[friends]、生涯 [career]、妻子 [wife]、房屋 [house], etc.

Proper Nouns

詹姆斯 [James]、約翰 [John]、馬克.吐溫 [Mark Twin]、波莉 [Polly]、瑪麗 [Mary]、湯姆 [Tom]、道格拉斯 [Douglas]、柴契爾 [Thatcher]、哈克.貝利 [Huck Belly], etc. 太平洋[the Specific Ocean]、西班牙[Spaniard]、阿拉伯 [Arab]、約克市 [the York city]、德國 [Germany]、土耳其 [Turkey]、法國 [France]、歐洲[Europe], etc.

Pronouns

[you]、我[me/I]、他[him/he]、她[her/she]、你們[you]、我們[we/us]、他們[them/they]

3.2.2. Connectives

Connectives that refer to conjunctions and transitional words are normally used to signify logical relations between two clauses within one sentence or between two sentences. The five types of logical connectives are temporal, contrastive, causal, coordinate and conditional. The addition of these connectives in the translation helps the target reader to catch the message easily because they may explicate the implicit logical relationship at the sentence level. Table 4 shows some of the selected five connective types.


Table 4

Five Connective Types

Connective Types

Lexical Items

Temporal Connectives

[when]、接著[after that]、然後[then]、不一會兒[after a while]、後來[later]

Contrastive Connectives

[but]、可是[however]、卻[but]、反而[on the contrary]、不過 [nevertheless]

Causal Connectives

因為[because]、由於[due to]、所以 [so]、於是[thus]、因此[therefore]、結果[as a result]

Coordinate Connectives

[and]、和 [an]、並[and]、並且 [and]、或 [or]

Conditional Connectives

假如 [if]、假設 [if]、假若[provided that]、雖然[although/though]、只要 [only if]

3.2.3. Added Notes

The names of some people or places in English novels have significant socio-cultural implications and provide non-English native children readers with useful knowledge and information. I randomly select some of them to check if they are translated with added notes. Table 5 shows these searched words that fall into six categories: place's names, people's names, foods, measurements, diseases, objects and religious terms.


Table 5

Six Categories of Cultural Notes

Six Categories

Lexical Items

Place's Names

Nottinghamshire, Emanuel College, Cambridge, Leyden, Levant, Old Jury, Fetter Lane, South Sea, Bristol, Newgate-street,, Van Diemen's Land, Burgundy, etc.

People's Names

Mr. James Bates, Captain Abraham Pannel, Mr. Edmund Burton, Mrs. Mary Burton, John, Captain William Prichard, Tom, Douglas, Miss Watson, Huckleberry, etc.

Foods

Brandy, doughnut, punch, rusk, bisket, cane, roots, corn, rum, etc.

Measurements

hogshead, latitude, league, eight, ducat, hundredweight, mile, pound, ounce, foot, etc.

Objects

Jerkin, musket bullets, signet royal, spear, pulley, snuff, harp, interest, Bible, pipe, carriage, ingots, julery (jewelry), hymn-book, Don Quixote, Small-shot, Foremast, etc.

Others

Stroke, Yellow Jack, Hangman, Walk the plank, etc.


IV. Findings

I used the Concordance tool to calculate noun density, and then obtained the following. Table 6 shows the difference in noun density between the literature translations for children and for adults.


Table 6

Noun Density Differences Between LPCC and LPCA

The Number of Nouns

The Number of Tokens

Percentages

LPCC

1737

7081

24%

LPCA

10010

36529

27%


Table 6 shows that LPCC has lower noun density and therefore uses fewer varied types of nouns than LPCA. Thus, the translation for children has a 3% higher explicitation degree at the lexical level than does the translation for adults. This discrepancy in explicitation is not large compared to the explicitation gap by connectives (22%) and the explicitation gap by notes (28%).

Additionally, the author used the ParaConc tool to analyze and calculate the added connectives, and then obtained the explicitation percentages (Table 7).


Table 7

Connectives-Resulted Explicitation Differences Between LPCC and LPCA

The Number of Added Connectives

The Number of All Connectives

Explicitation Percentages

LPCC

92

213

43%

LPCA

246

1126

21%


Table 7 reveals that the degree of explicitation resulting from added connectives in translated literature for children is 22% higher than that for adults. However, this gap is still smaller than the explicitation by notes, as shown in Table 8.

Table 8

Notes-Resulted Explicitation Differences Between LPCC and LPCA

The Number of Added Notes

The Number of Nouns

Explicitation Percentages

LPCC

52

98

53%

LPCA

68

272

25%

Table 8 shows that literature translation for children shows a much higher percentage of explicitation resulting from added notes than the translation for adults. This finding suggests that the addition of cultural notes is a crucial strategy in the translation for the younger audience. The combination of lower noun density and higher percentages of added connectives and added cultural notes justifies a higher degree of explicitation in the translated novels for children than for adults.


V. Discussions and Implications

The above-mentioned statistical figures reveal some significant insights and these points will be discussed in the areas of linguistic effect vs. aesthetic effect, educational function vs. literary function, and communicative acceptability vs. literal adequacy.


5.1 Linguistic Effect vs. Aesthetic Effect

In response to the first research question, the result of the investigation indicates that noun density is slightly lower in the translated novels for children (24%) than for adults (27%). Since noun density is a method of measuring the lexical complexity of a text, the lower density in the translated novels for children reveals the use of fewer types of nouns. The repetitive use of certain lexical items might reduce the number of noun types in the translation for children. For example, the English phrases "any thing," "Toys and Trifles," and "my new gain'd wealth" are all translated with the same Chinese word, "/貨物" [goods] in a translated novel for children, while these words are respectively translated as "什麼貨物" [what things], "小掛件和小擺設" [small pendants and small decorations], and "新掙到手的錢"[money newly earned in the hands] in the translated novel for adults. More interestingly, "Ben Rogers" and "little Tommy Barnes" are both translated as "孩子" [child/ hei-ze] for children, but are literally translated as "班.羅傑斯" [ban luo jie si] and "小湯米.巴恩斯"[xiao tang mi ba en si] for adults. The use of rich vocabulary in the translation for adults does not create the comprehensibility burden but it would probably interfere with children's learning of the target language. Table 9 provides more examples of the repetitive use of some lexical items in the literature translation for children.


Table 9

A Comparison of Different Noun Translations for Children and for Adults

Original English Sentences

Chinese Translations for Children

Chinese Translations for Adults

His excellency, having mounted on my small right leg, advanced forwards up to my face, with about a dozen of his retinue.

一個欽差大臣便帶著一打隨員,從我的右小腿那裡走了上來。

[A government-appointed official moved forward up from my small right leg/my translation.]

這位大官帶了十幾位隨從,沿著我的右小腿彎處爬上來,走到我的臉前面。

[This official and a dozen of his retinue climbed up from my small right leg and advanced forwards up to my face/my translation.]

For he shook his head by way of disapprobation, and held his hand in a posture to show that I must be carried as a prisoner.

欽差大臣只是猛地搖頭,並用手勢告訴我,一定要把我當俘虜運走。

[The government-appointed official shook his head vehemently and used his gesture to suggest that he would carry me away as a prisoner/my translation.]

搖頭表示不同意,還舉起手做了個手勢,說明非得把我當俘虜運走。

[He shook his head to show his disapproval and held his hand as a gesture to denote that he would carry me away as a prisoner/my translation.]

Upon this, the HURGO and his train withdrew, with much civility and cheerful countenances.

欽差大臣退下去了。

[The government-appointed official withdrew/my translation.]

這樣「赫哥」和隨從才彬彬有禮、和顏悅色地退下。

[Thus, the HURGO and his train withdrew, with much civility and cheerful countenances/my translation.]

Table 9 shows that the English entries such as "His excellency," "he" and HURGO" are all translated as "欽差大臣"[the government-appointed official], but are respectively translated as "這位大官"[this high-ranking official], "" and "赫哥" [HURGO] for adults. The repeated translation of "欽差大臣"[the government-appointed official] impresses young readers and enables them to memorize it easily. Repeating a translated term also helps children to acquire new words in a more impressive way. This lingustic impact meets Shavit's calling for the adjustment of children's literature for easy comprehension and easy learning.

In contrast, the linguistic impact resulting from the repetitive translation is useless for adult readers. Adults require literal translation with higher lexical variation because most of them are eager for aesthetic appreciation. Adult readers with higher TL proficiency accept lexical complexity in translation and tolerate any subtle presentation. The rich vocabulary for adults does not create a comprehensibility burden but it would probably impair children's language learning. Some adult readers favour high lexical variation and view this as derived from the translator's literary style. As stated earlier by Shavit, the translation for adults aims to produce literary beauty and boost aesthetic effect, so it expects to use a higher variety of lexical items.

In short, the higher lexical density creates aesthetic values for adults, whereas the lower lexical density brings the benefits to easy comprehension and easy memorization to children. The use of different linguistic techniques lead to the higher lexicon-specific explcitation for children than for adults. This explicitaiton variation concurs with Vermeer's skopos theory that every translation must be assigned a specific purpose and different translation strategies are used to achieve these intended purposes.


5.2 Communicative Acceptability vs. Literal Adequacy

With regard to the second research question, the result of investigation informs us that translated novels for children have a higher frequency of added connectives than those for adults do. Thus, a higher degree of explicitation at the syntactic level can be found in the translation for children.

The addition of connectives explicates the implicit logical relations between two clauses and clarifies the core message of the sentence for child readers. Because of children's weak logical sense, added connectives assist in their understanding of the contextual relations. Let us compare the following pairs of translations in Table 10.


Table 10

A Comparison of Added Connectives in the Translations for Children and for Adults

Original English Sentences

Chinese Translations for Children

Chinese Translations for Adults

I was extremely tired, and with that, and the heat of the weather, and about half a pint of brandy that I drank as I left the ship, I found myself much inclined to sleep

由於虛弱和疲乏,我倒在草地上就呼呼地睡著了,我一輩子都沒有睡過這麼酣甜的覺。

[(Because) I was extremely weak and tired, I lay on the grassland and immediately fell into sleep; I never had such a deep and sweet sleep in my lifetime/my translation.]

__我疲憊不堪,加上天氣炎熱,離開大船前喝了約半品脫白蘭地,睡意強烈來襲。

[____I was extremely tired, and with the heat of the weather, I drank about half a pint of brandy as I left the ship, henceforth finding myself much inclined to sleep/my translation.

We rowed, by my computation, about three leagues, till we were able to work no longer, being already spent with labor while we were in the ship.

划出三海里遠後,就再也划不動了,因為大家已經筋疲力盡了,只得聽任海浪擺布。

[However, after rowing three leagues, we were unable to row any more because we were exhausted and were only subject to the sway of sea.]

__據我推算,因為已在大船上耗盡氣力,我們大約划了三英里便再也划不動。

___I speculated that because we already spent with labor in the ship, we were able to work no longer after we rowed three leagues.]

She said all a body would have to do there was to go around all day long with a harp and sing, forever and ever.

接著她就把天堂的情景描述了一番,說在天堂裡是不用工作的,只不過到處走走,彈彈琴,唱唱歌,就這樣永遠永遠地過下去。

[Next, she described the scene of the heaven where nobody had to work, simply fooling around, playing the harp and singing song, living on in such a way forever and ever.]

____她說人在那裡整天不用作別的事,只需要抱著豎琴彈琴歌唱,讚美永不止息。

[__She said that people there did nothing all the day, simply singing song carrying a harp, and compliments never stopped.]

With the added conjunction because and the transitional words however and next, children will find it is easier to catch the cause-effect, comparison-contrast and chronological relations implicit in the context, and therefore are able to catch the message quickly. Added connectives help achieve the communication effect for children, but added connectives are of little help for adults who are more capable of seeing the implicit logical relations between sentences or between two clauses of one sentence. Added connectives sometimes destroy the literary sense and feeling of the original work. Thus, it is advisable to use literal translation method when translating novels for adults.

The difference between syntactic adaptation for children and literal translation for adults aims to fulfill different translation purposes: a contrast between communicative acceptability and information adequacy. This contrastive concept was raised by G. Toury when he discussed the issue of translational equivalence. In Toury's viewpoint, translation involves "an encounter, if not a confrontation, between two sets of norms" (1980, p. 55). When translation adheres to the norms of the SL system, the result is literal adequacy. Nevertheless, when translation follows the norms of the TL system, it brings about communicative acceptability. Thus, the adapted version fits young readers for the purpose of effective communication, while literal translation is suitable for adult readers for the purpose of informational adequacy. In the meantime, this discrimination conforms to Vermeer's skopos theory that every translation behavior is aimed at accomplishing a specific purpose.


5.3 Educational Function vs. Literary Function

Regarding the third research question, the answer is that translated novels for children have a higher frequency of added notes than translated novels for adults do. This means a higher degree of explicitation at the contextual level in the translation for children than for adults.

The purpose of adding notes is to transmit useful knowledge or background information of places, names, foods and others to the audience. Young readers, particularly elementary school students, are curious about and need to acquire cultural information or specialized knowledge about the outside world. At this point, young readers can access the deeper aspects of the foreign culture and acquire some specialized knowledge if they read the translation of foreign novels dotted with cultural or specialized notes. They can expand their cultural and informational horizons. The higher frequency of notes aims to achieve the purpose that the translation for children should be educational (Shavit 1986). Table 11 shows the different translations with and without cultural and specialized notes


Table 11

A Comparison of Added Notes in the Translations for Children and for Adults

Original English Sentences

Chinese Translations for Children

Chinese Translations for Adults

...rendered it necessary that I should go to Paris, there to communicate with a gentleman of the Bank, so good as to be dispatched to Paris for the purpose.

「信上說,讓我和銀行派來的一位先生接洽,然後到巴黎去。」[The letter said that they let me communicate with a gentleman assigned by the Bank, so that I went to Paris/my translation.]

: 巴黎,法國首都巴黎,建立在塞納河畔,是世界著名的大都市和旅遊勝地,艾菲爾鐵塔、凱旋門、香榭大道、博物館、美術館,每年吸引無數的遊客前往觀光。

[Note: Paris, the capital city of France, is situated on the River Seine as one of the worldwide famous big cities and scenic resorts. Eiffel Tower, Arc de Triomphe, Champs Élysées, museums, and galleries make Paris an attraction for numerous foreign tourists per year/my translation. ]

「因此我必須去巴黎一趟,和一位銀行的先生聯絡,他也是為此而奉派到巴黎。」[I have to go to Paris to communicate with a gentleman who was also appointed to Paris for this matter/my translation.]

PS: No notes for Paris.

By an observation, we found ourselves in the latitude of 30 degrees 2 minutes south.

約在南三十度零二分。

[About in the latitude of 30 degrees 2 minutes south/my translation]

註:,指緯線。地圖上所有與赤道平行的較小圓周線,都稱為緯線。赤道以北,稱北緯;以南,稱南緯。[Note: Latitude means line of latitude. Line of latitude refers to the small circles parallel to the equator in the map. North latitude is located to the north of the equator; south latitude, to the south of the equator/my translation.]

根據觀測,我們發現自已的位置在南三十度零二分。[According to our observation, we found we were located in the latitude of 30 degrees 2 minutes south/my translation.]

PS: No notes for latitude.

And she took snuff, too; of course that was all right, because she done it herself.

而她卻在聞鼻煙,這反而是合情合理的。[And she took snuff and this was reasonably acceptable/my translation.]

註:鼻煙,用鼻吸的煙。鼻煙放在鼻煙壺內,吸用時用小勺舀出放在食指上,靠近鼻孔吸入。

[Note: Snuff refers to smokeless tobacco for sniffing. Snuff stored in a bottle can be put on the index finger for sniffing through the nose/my translation.]

她自己也吸鼻菸,當然這下就沒什麼不對了,因為吸菸的可是她自己嘛。 [She herself took snuff and there was nothing wrong with this because she did it herself/my translation.]

PS: No notes for snuff.


To consider the added note about Paris, for example, it helps children readers to know the location and cultural importance of France's most famous city. The added notes of latitude and snuff provide children with specialized knowledge of geography and smokeless tobacco.

Adult readers, however, have already heard of Paris and have known well what latitude refers to or know how to find information about snuff, so additional notes about these terms are not necessary and are not added in the translation for adults. So far, we have known that added notes may provide children with cultural input through access to cultural information and specialized knowledge, but these notes might be redundant and ruin the original aesthetic form for adults. The differences in explicitation resulting from added notes between the translations for children and for adults meets Shavit's (1986) argument that children's literature is educational, but adult literature has a literary and aesthetic function.


VI. Conclusion

This corpus-based study with statistical analysis has proved that the degree of explicitation varies between translations of children and adults' literature. Translated novels for children have a lower noun density (24%), a higher frequency of added connectives (43%) and a higher frequency of added notes (53%), so they show a higher degree of explicitaiton. In contrast, translated novels for adults have a higher noun density (27%), a lower frequency of added connectives (21%) and a lower frequency of added notes (25%), leading to a lower degree of explicitation. The hidden reasons for this discrepancy are that translated novels for children emphasize easy-to-understand lexical presentation with effective linguistic input while the translated novels for adults seek more complex lexical presentation for aesthetic appreciation. Furthermore, children require adapted translation for effective communication while adults require literal translation for informational adequacy. Finally, children's literature is an educational tool, requiring easier-to-learn lexicons and additional information input, but adult literature has a literary purpose, pursuing spiritual amusement and sublimation. In summary, the explication variation has close relevance to the target audience, the reading purpose and the translation function.

Although this study is limited in its corpora size (132,561 words in total), it serves as a model of purpose-oriented explicitation study using the corpus-based analysis method. It may be used as a point of departure for future researches on other literary genres, e.g. science fiction, in other language pairs to gain more significant insights.


Endnotes

1 A parallel corpus is made up of a set of texts in language A and their translations into language B. We must distinguish the parallel corpus from the comparable corpus that is composed of the source language text and its corresponding target language text. The parallel corpus is the very corpus type that "one immediately thinks of in the context of translation studies" (Baker 1995, p. 230). This type of corpus can be adopted to investigate a variety of shifts that occur in the process of translation, such as addition of conjunctions, pronominal restoration, and insertion of extra background information.

2 Noun density is actually the type-token ratio. The number of noun types refers to the number of the selected nouns while tokens comprise the total number of word forms in the text. For example, the word "歷史" (history) occurs 11 times in the translated Chinese text and represents only one noun type, but it constitutes 11 tokens. The noun density is calculated by dividing the number of tokens by the number of queried nouns.

3 The ParaCon tool is used to develop the parallel corpus by aligning the SL and the TL texts. It can also be used to obtain the concordance output through the query for the key words listed in the previous section. For example, when we examine addition of logical connectives as an explicitation strategy in TL texts, we could use the Parallel Search function, supported by the ParaConc tool, to search for ""[but] in the TL text. The translated sentences that contain "but," but are not found in their corresponding English sentences show the explicitation tendency. Besides, a Cisco Key Integrity Protocol (C KIP) system is used to segment the translated Chinese text into small linguistic units and label their parts of speech for easier identification. Finally, the Concordance tool is used to obtain the statistical figures of the queried nouns and tokens (known as all running words).

4 All five translated novels for children were published by Tien-Wei Publishing Company in 1996 in Taiwan.

5 The five novels translated for adults were released on the market between 2001 and 2006, with《雙城記》published by Wisdom Books Co., Ltd. in 2001;《格列佛遊記》and《頑童流浪記》published by Business Weekly Publications, Inc. in 2005;《金銀島》published by Elegant Books Cultural Enterprise Co., Ltd. in 2006, and《魯賓遜漂流記》published by Strom & Stress Publishing Company in 2006.


References

Baker, M (1993). Corpus Linguistics and Translation Studies: Implications and Applications. In M. Baker, G. Francis & E. Tognini-Bonelli (Eds.), Test and technology: In honour of John Sinclair (pp. 223-250). Philadelphia/Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Baker, M (1995). Corpora in Translation Studies: An overview and some suggestions for future research. Target 7(2), 223-243.

Baker, M (1996). Corpus-based Translation Studies: The Challenges that Lie Ahead. In H. Somers (Ed.), Terminology, LSP and translation studies in language engineering: In honor of Juan C. Sager (pp. 175-186). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

Baker, M. (2001). Routledge Encyclopedia of Translation Studies. London and New York: Routledge.

Blum-Kulka, S. ([1986]2000). Shifts of Cohesion and Coherence in Translation. In L. Venuti (Ed.), The translation studies reader (pp. 298-313). London and New York: Routledge.

Bowker, L. (2002). Computer-aided Translation Technology. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press.

Bowker, L. (1999). Exploring the Potential of Corpora for Raising Language Awareness in Student Translators. Language Awareness (8), 160-73.

Gavioli, L. & Zanettin, F. (1997). Comparable Corpora and Translation: A pedagogic perspective. In G. Aston, L. Gavioli & F. Zanettin (Eds.), Corpus use and learning to translate. Retrieved Dec. 21, 2006 from http://www.sslmit.unibo.it/cultpaps/

Klaudy, K. (1998). Explicitation. In M. Baker (Ed.), Routledge Encyclopedia of Translation Studies (pp. 80-84). London & New York: Routledge.

Laviosa, Sara (2002). Corpus-based Translation sStudies, Theory, Findings, Applications. Amsterdam: Rodopi.

Munday, J. (2001). Introducing Translation Studies: Theories and applications. London: Routledge.

Mutesayire, M. (2005). Cohesive Devices and Explicitation in Translated English: A corpus-based study. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of Manchester.

Nida, Eugene A. (1964). Toward a Science of Translating: With Special Reference to Principles and Procedures Involved in Bible Translating. Leiden: E. J. Brill.

Olohan, M., & Baker, M. (2000). Reporting "that" in Translated English: Evidence for Subliminal Processes of Explicitation? Across Languages and Cultures, 1(2), 141-158

Olohan, M. (2003). How Frequent are the Contractions? A study of contracted forms in the translational English corpus. Target (15), 59-89.

Øverås, L. (1998). In Search of the Third Code: An investigation of norms in literary translation. Meta, 43(4), 571-588

Reiss, Katharina & Vermeer, H. J. (1984). Grundlegung einer allgemeinen Translationstheorie. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

Shavit, Zohar (1986). Poetics of Children's Literature. Athens and London: The University of George Press.

Shuttleworth, M. & Cowie, M. (1997) Dictionary of Translation Studies. Manchester, UK.: St. Jerome Publishing.

Stewart, D. (2000). Conventionality, Creativity and Translated Text: The implications of electronic corpora in translation. In M. Olohan (Ed.). Intercultural Faultlines. Research Methods in Translation Studies I: Textual and cognitive aspects (pp. 73-91). Manchester: St. Jerome. .

Toury, Gideon (1980). In Search of a Theory of Translation. Tel Aviv: The Porter Institute for Poetics and Semiotics.

Vanderauwera, R. (1985). Dutch Novels Translated into English: The transformation of a "minority" literature. Amsterdam: Rodopi.

Vermeer, H. J. ([1989]2000). Skopos and Commission in Translational Action (A. Chesterman, Trans.). In L. Venuti (Ed.), The translation studies reader (pp.221-232). London and New York: Routledge.

Vinay, J.-P., & Darbelnet, J. ([1958/1995]2000). A Methodology for Translation (J. C. Sager & M.-J. Hamel, Trans.). In L. Venuti (Ed.), The Translation studies reader (pp. 84-93). London and New York: Routledge.

Zanettin, E. (2000). Parallel Corpora in Translation Studies: Issues in corpus design. In M. Olohan (Ed.). Intercultural faultlines. Research methods in translation studies I: Textual and cognitive aspects (pp. 105-18). Manchester: St. Jerome. .


Acknowledgements

I extend my enormous thanks to two graduate students, Vicky and Ruby, at the National Kaohsiung First University of Science and Technology, for helping me develop two parallel corpora. I also thank Taiwan's National Science Council for granting me funds to complete this project.